- Most cannabis studies are double-blind trials, which are used in medical experiments to prevent participants from knowing if they got a placebo
- In studies of cannabis with THC, participants often know they got the active treatment because they feel its euphoric effect, or 'high'
- One doctor argues that this means that researchers are overestimating the effectiveness of cannabis to treat medical issues like pain or depression
Some cannabis
studies may overstate the substance's benefits because participants know
when they are 'high', one scientist argues.
Countless studies have shown that active cannabinoids can effectively treat everything from chronic pain to depression and PTSD - at least according to high people.
Most
cannabis studies are double-blind trials, meaning that neither
researchers nor participants know which of two groups got a sample
containing active cannabinoids, and which got an otherwise identical,
inactive substance.
But when feeling
'high' tips off participants in studies using THC - the cannabis
chemical the creates that euphoric feeling - the results become skewed,
argues one Duke University medical professor.
Cannabis studies may be
overestimating its medical benefits because participants know when they
are getting 'high,' a Duke University professor argues
Medical marijuana is now legal 29 states in the US.
The
legalization movement rode heavily on scientific evidence that
cannabinoids - the active portion of the cannabis plant from which
marijuana, hashish and hash oil are made - are safe and have have health
benefits.
Studies on the therapeutic uses of cannabis have largely followed the gold standard of medical research: double-blind trials.
Double-blind
trials are meant to eliminate the bias of participants who don't know
if they were given a real treatment or a placebo.
But Duke University professor Dr David Casarett says that cannabis experiments don't control for the high bias.
Most studies on cannabis have participants either smoke or inhale vaporized versions of the plant derivative.
In these studies, researchers make a
control by extracting cannabinoids from cannabis. The resulting product
tastes, smells and looks identical, but has none of the psychoactive or
physical effects of marijuana or other active forms of cannabis.
In
his JAMA Internal Medicine article, Dr Casarett cites one study on
cannabis for pain treatment in which all but one of the 15 participants
correctly guessed that they'd been given the active form of cannabis.
In another, people that got the real cannabis reported feeling 'high,' 'stoned,' or 'sedated.'
'These
participants guess their group assignment in large part because they
detect the psychoactive properties of the active cannabis treatment,'
he writes.
In other words, the studies aren't actually all that blind.
When
blinding fails, participants are more likely to say they've experienced
beneficial effects from an active treatment because they believe that
they should.
'These effects should raise substantial concerns over the validity of medical cannabis trials,' Dr Casarett writes.
The 'high' associated with cannabis comes from THC, one of the chemicals in cannabinoids.
Dr Cassaret only discusses trials that use cannabinoids that still have THC in them, but not all studies do.
In
many studies, particularly on cannabis's effects on physical pain,
researchers remove THC from the samples they use so that participants
are only getting cannabidiol, or CBD.
CBD does not affect the same receptors as THC, so it doesn't create the euphoric 'high' feeling many associate with cannabis.
The effects of CBD are not entirely understood, but it is thought to counteract inflammation in the brain and body.
Dr
Casarett suggests that including another psychoactive ingredient in
controls, but admits that 'unfortunately, it is not clear what an ideal
psychoactive control would be in a cannabis trial.'
Substitutions
would have to create euphoria, dry mouth and raise heart rates without
providing any of the forms of relief being studied.
Dr
Casarett also considers recruiting only people who have never used any
form of cannabis and do not know firsthand what its effects feel like.
Other
studies, however, have shown that some of the effects of cannabinoids,
particularly elevated heart rates are actually most pronounced in people
who have never used any form of cannabis.
Regardless, he calls for cannabis studies to acknowledge this limitation.
Otherwise,
'there is a very real risk that physicians and patients will
misinterpret the results of these studies and see benefits that do not
exist,' he writes.
No comments:
Post a Comment