Secondhand exposure to cannabis smoke under "extreme conditions," such as an unventilated room
or enclosed vehicle, can cause nonsmokers to feel the effects of the
drug, have minor problems with memory and coordination, and in some
cases test positive for the drug in a urinalysis. Those are the findings
of a Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine study, reported online
this month in the journal Drug and alcohol Dependence.
Cannabis is the most widely used illicit drug in the world. "Many
people are exposed to secondhand cannabis smoke," says lead author Evan
S. Herrmann, Ph.D., a postdoctoral fellow in psychiatry and behavioral sciences
at Johns Hopkins. "The scenario we looked at was almost a worst-case
scenario. It could happen in the real world, but it couldn't happen to
someone without him or her being aware of it."
"We found positive drug effects in the first few hours, a mild sense
of intoxication and mild impairment on measures of cognitive
performance," says senior author Ryan Vandrey, Ph.D. , an associate
professor of psychiatry and behavioral sciences at Johns Hopkins. "These
were relatively slight effects, but even so, some participants did not pass the equivalent of a workplace drug test."
The new
research is the most comprehensive study of secondhand cannabis smoke
and its effects since the 1980s, when researchers found the drug's active ingredient, tetrahydrocannabinol, or THC, and other cannabis byproducts could turn up in nonsmokers'
bodies after an hour or more spent in extreme conditions with heavy
smokers in an enclosed space. That finding needed updating, since the
average potency of street cannabis has tripled since the 1980s, the
Johns Hopkins researchers wrote. Additionally, many earlier studies did
not look at whether the nonsmokers reported feeling the drug's effects,
or whether their behavior and thinking were affected by secondhand smoke, as the new study did.
Researchers recruited seven people ages 18 to 45 who said they smoked
cannabis at least twice per week and tested positive for THC, but who
tested negative for other drugs, and 12 others in the same age range who
said they had not used cannabis in the past six months and tested
negative for cannabis, other drugs, and alcohol. None of the
participants were pregnant, and none of the nonsmokers took part in more
than one session.
Six smokers and six nonsmokers spent an hour sitting side by side in a
10-by-13-foot, acrylic-walled room in two different experimental
sessions. Each smoker was given 10 high-potency cannabis cigarettes to
smoke. In one session, the room's
ventilation fans were turned on. In another session, the fans were
turned off, and the room became smoke-filled. This was a realistic
simulation of home ventilation conditions. At the end of the exposures,
smokers' and nonsmokers' blood, urine, saliva and hair were tested at
regular intervals for THC.
All six nonsmokers who spent an hour exposed to secondhand smoke in the unventilated room under extreme conditions
had detectable amounts of THC in their urine and blood. THC in blood
was observed immediately after exposure and for up to three hours
afterwards. Four hours after the experiment ended, one nonsmoker tested
positive for THC on a urine test with the same cutoff (50 nanograms per
milliliter) used in the Federal Workplace Drug Testing Program. At
intervals between two and 22 hours after the experiment, four of the six
nonsmokers tested positive for THC in their urine at a lower cutoff (20
nanograms per milliliter) sometimes used in commercial drug testing
programs.
None of the nonsmokers exposed to secondhand smoke in the ventilated
room tested positive for THC on either the more sensitive or the less
sensitive urinalysis. (All the cannabis smokers tested positive for THC
afterward.)
Nonsmokers exposed to secondhand smoke with fans running reported no
effects other than being hungry. Those who were exposed in the
unventilated experiment reported feeling "pleasant," more tired and less
alert. When the nonsmokers were asked to duplicate grid patterns they
saw on a computer monitor or perform a basic numbers drill, those in the
unventilated study responded faster but made more mistakes than they
did before they were exposed to the cannabis smoke, the researchers
found.
"The behavioral and cognitive effects were minor and consistent with a mild cannabis effect," Herrmann says.
"This study is a significant update in our knowledge of cannabis smoke
effect on nonsmokers and has implications in many arenas, including
drugs and driving," says co-author Edward J. Cone, Ph.D., a Johns
Hopkins adjunct professor of psychiatry and behavioral sciences who
performed the early passive inhalation studies in the 1980s.
The study's limitations included its small size and the lack of a
placebo trial using cannabis that contained no THC. The study was
supported by the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services
Administration (SAMHSA), an agency in the U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services that sets standards for federal workplace drug testing.
Information about the effects of secondhand smoke on drug test results
were being sought to support different ways of measuring drug use or drug
exposure, Vandrey says. Funding came from SAMHSA and the National
Center for Research Resources of the National Institutes of Health. The cannabis used in the experiment was provided by the National Institute on Drug Abuse.
No comments:
Post a Comment